Contra Pfizer

My name is Lothar Schröder, I am German and 45 years old. I studied mathematics and actuaries at Cologne University. After graduation I have worked for several insurance companies as a fixed income portfolio manger. Now I work for a bank.
I am a widower since my wife Monika killed herself three years ago.

My wife took ZOLOFT, an SSRI-antidepressant manufactured by the pharma company Pfizer. At the time she took the drug, the package insert did not warn that ZOLOFT can make people suicidal. I blame the drug for her death, because she loved live and would never have done that. She had so many plans for the future. The medical documents show that it is very likely, that there was a causal relationship between the drug ZOLOFT and her suicide.

The risk of suicide was concealed in Germany by Pfizer until August 2005, when the European Commission forced the pharmaceutical companies like Pfizer to warn about the suicide risk for children and adolescents. Some weeks ago, Pfizer has increased the age at higher risk from 18 years to 25 years. But this is nonsense. All ages are at higher risk from these SSRI- Antidepressants!
Other SSRI antidepressants are Prozac (in Germany Fluctin) and Paxil. They all carry the same risk!
This blog is about these SSRI- antidepressants and CONTRA PFIZER.
It is named after one article by Dr. David Healy who has written a book on SSRI, it is titled “Let them eat Prozac”.
I have decided to fight Pfizer and tell my story to the public.

Mittwoch, 4. Dezember 2013

Drug Trials and Data-Based Medicine: An Interview with David Healy

(Quelle: http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/side-effects/201207/drug-trials-and-data-based-medicine-interview-david-healy)



Dr. David Healy is an internationally renowned psychiatrist, psychopharmacologist, scientist, and author. A professor of Psychiatry in Wales and former Secretary of the British Association for Psychopharmacology, he is the author of more than 150 peer-reviewed articles and 20 books, including The Antidepressant Era and The Creation of Psychopharmacology, from Harvard University Press; Let Them Eat Prozac from New York University Press; Mania: A Short History of Bipolar Disorder from the Johns Hopkins University Press; and, most recently, Pharmageddon, from the University of California Press. He was responsible for submitting the key document that led to New York State's successful fraud action against GlaxoSmithKline, a key plank in the Department of Justice's recent case against the drug-maker.

David, thanks for answering a few questions about your latest work. With a team of other medical specialists you recently launched a new website, RxISK, that provides a wealth of user-friendly drug-related information for doctors and patients. What distinguishes RxISK from other sites listing medical information and what were some of your aims in launching it?

Chris, we’re trying to provide much-better descriptions of drug-related side effects, including by getting patients and doctors to work as teams and by using a series of causality algorithms that help establish when there’s a link between a treatment and a problem.
Other sites providing medical information tend to offer one of two things: either they summarize clinical trials, most of which are ghostwritten and where the complete data are unavailable, or they list adverse event data from agencies like the FDA, which is of poor quality and increasingly regarded as anecdotal. Regulators and academics now have a track-record of acknowledging significant hazards on drugs 10-20 years after patients and others first draw attention to them. The reason that happens is because agencies like the FDA have degraded adverse event reporting.
I expect we’ll get reports from people like you and your readers outlining a new problem on some new drug that clears when the drug is stopped and maybe reappears if it’s restarted. Companies and academics will then scream blue murder—this is just anecdotal.
My response will be to ask whether that report is more or less likely to be correct than clinical trial data run by companies whose data are hidden, and where the patients sometimes don’t even exist. Even where the data prove beyond doubt that the drug causes the problem, academics unfortunately will still line up to deny that the drug could cause it.

You’ve written on your blog that “evidence-based medicine and RCTs [random controlled trials] are supposed to help us control the pharmaceutical industry.” Yet “RCTs are simply not the answer to determining cause and effect,” you go on to say, because they’re “quite likely to hide rather than reveal a problem like antidepressant induced suicidality.” As one of the first researchers to draw attention to the now well-publicized suicide-inducing side effects of many antidepressants, you’re clearly in a position to answer: how in fact do RCTs hide such information?

There are a few ways that RCTs can hide effects. First, the process doesn’t encourage anyone to look closely at particular things that happen on a drug—the focus is instead on the group and on average effects. That’s true of all trials. In company trials there are more specific problems like miscoding, where suicidality becomes “nausea” or “emotional lability” or even “treatment non-responsiveness.” There is also the problem of mislocation—patients on placebo end up being given problems they never had—and of nonexistent patients, who don’t of course have adverse events.
Beyond that, there are more sophisticated tricks that companies can and do play—such as claiming that increased rates of a problem on a drug are not really evidence of an increase in rates if the data are not statistically significant. In this way, companies have hidden many more heart attacks on Vioxx and Avandia or suicidal acts on SSRIs than have been hidden by miscoding or mislocation.


Isn’t what you’re describing tantamount to fraud? I’m all in favor of clinical trials—if done right, wouldn’t they give us the correct answer?

Actually no, when it comes to adverse events, trials almost never get the right answer.
Let’s assume in a trial that we have 3,000 depressed patients on Paxil who had 10 suicidal acts and 1,750 on placebo who had 0 suicidal acts. Paxil clearly causes suicidal acts here. Now let’s take 200 depressive personality disorder patients on Paxil who have 30 suicidal acts and 200 depressive personality disorder patients on placebo who have 25 suicidal acts—again, that’s an increased rate of suicidal acts on Paxil. But add these two increases together and you end up with a reduced rate of suicidal acts on the SSRI compared to placebo—40 suicidal acts in 3,200 patients is less than 25 in 1,950.
Hey presto—problem gone. Exactly the same thing can happen in every clinical trial where we don’t fully understand the condition we’re treating—which is, frankly, most conditions from back pain to diabetes to psychosis. We mix patients who superficially appear the same but who in fact have different conditions.
That is just one trick that no-one ever mentions—I’ve laid out several more on davidhealy.org.


Is there any way to overcome such tricks and masking problems?

Yes, actually, there is. One way is to do trials in healthy volunteers—these are the true drug trials. Companies do these but rarely publish them. There’s no register of these trials and no data are made available, though there’s no issue of clinical confidentiality involved. Given that these trials tell us so much—10 years before Zoloft came on the market, for instance, they indicated that the drug made healthy volunteers suicidal—it’s a huge scandal that these data in particular are buried.

Speaking out against trials won’t of course make you popular. 

It certainly doesn’t. But the most painful thing of all is it puts me at odds with almost everyone who should be a natural ally: those who are committed to Evidence-Based Medicine, some of whom agree with what is being said, or who even claim that I am saying nothing new, but who really don’t want to see RCTs questioned in public and can get unpleasantly angry when they are.

Is there a chance that RxISK will represent data from other countries, to establish a truly global perspective on adverse effects from medication?

Absolutely—RxISK will have data from every country under the sun and will also breakdown the data by locality so, for instance, people in Chicago will be able to see what side effects are being reported on which drug in their area. This may well be of interest to journalists who want local stories—if in this case a large urban area can be called local.
You’ll also be able to follow your side effect across time—how common it becomes, where it’s being reported most, who gets it—men, women, young, old, etc. Given the input of hundreds of thousands of people following these things, I expect the visualization involved will help researchers come up with good ideas about what might actually be going on.

RxISK dovetails with—indeed, is clearly a practical extension of—the argument of your latest book Pharmageddon, that medicine has become increasingly “pharmaceuticalized” since the 1950s—oriented to fierce marketing campaigns that cherry-pick data, overhyph the overall benefits of drugs, and mask their very real hazards. This is obviously a timely argument in the States right now, with the Supreme Court’s decision on the Affordable Care Act and the debate about how to trim costs without affecting care. What (beyond your new site) are some of your recommendations for reforming healthcare and improving drug safety?

Well, the website is a bottom-up approach, a wisdom-of-crowds or bidet approach.
There are also top-down or shower approaches that could help. Our current problems stem tragically, in the proper sense of that word, from a system we put in place 50 years ago, following the thalidomide disaster, in an attempt to prevent such a problem happening again.
There are three components to the system—the patent status of drugs; the prescription-only status of drugs; and the issue of demonstrating efficacy through controlled trials. All need review to see whether some tweak to the system might produce better results than we are getting now.

Despite recent congressional attempts to create greater transparency over pharmaceutical decisions, such as Sen. Chuck Grassley’s Sunshine Act, you clearly are skeptical in the book that they’ve had much good effect as reforms. Part of the problem is clearly the FDA’s reliance on RCTs and a presumption that, as you put it, “takes for granted that data don’t lie.” What’s wrong, in your opinion, with the way the FDA currently interprets data?

Well, let’s run a thought-experiment and bring alcohol or nicotine on the market as antidepressants.
To do this, we don’t have to show lives saved or people returning to work—we only have to show a change in score on rating scales that may be sensitive to the anxiolytic or sedative effects of alcohol.
Next, we need do only a few studies in which alcohol beats placebo on our rating scale. If in most of our studies it doesn’t beat placebo, then these are discounted and FDA is happy for us to conceal that.
In our trials, placebo might account for 80-90% of the effect of alcohol but the FDA is fine with us leaving the public with the impression that 100% of the apparent benefits of alcohol for depression stem from the alcohol, with no contribution from placebo.
Better again, we can outsource our studies. Let’s say in our key study that alcohol proves no better than placebo in 30 U.S. centers, but is dramatically better than placebo in 2 Mexican centers, so, when added to the mix, alcohol marginally beats placebo. The FDA let’s us do this and the published article will make no mention that alcohol only works in Mexico.

What about side effects—could the FDA do more to improve drug safety?

The FDA couldn’t do worse. Our alcohol studies only have to last six-to-eight weeks and, as most of us know, few of the problems that might be expected from alcohol emerge in a six-to-eight week period.
If there’s any hint of liver problems in our trial, the FDA and academia are likely to attribute that to the depression for which the person is being treated. Even though the entire medical literature up till then might not have a scrap of evidence that depression causes liver dysfunction, within weeks companies have the ability to get a significant proportion of the medical profession to agree that it’s well-known that depression causes liver dysfunction.
Something else that’s extraordinary from a safety point of view is this: Several different companies can file for patents on whiskey, gin, brandy, wine, or port, or even to distinguish Irish whiskey from Scottish scotch. Their combined marketing can encourage doctors to put patients on combinations of whiskey, gin, brandy, and port and to keep their patients on these combinations for extended or indefinite periods of time.
If you or I had the power that Pharma has, we’d be able to get independent guidelines to endorse alcohol for depression, making it almost mandatory for doctors to use it.

Where are the AMA and APA in all of this?

This is where things get weird. The major difference between alcohol and Lexapro or Abilify lies in a curious inversion of the stranger-neighbor phenomenon. Rather stupidly, we are wary of strangers but comfortable with neighbors, even though we’re most likely to be harmed by neighbors or relatives.
Now, alcohol should be the familiar neighbor and SSRIs the dangerous stranger. But in fact we treat alcohol as a dangerous stranger, ripping a glass of wine out of the hands of a pregnant woman, while we regard SSRIs as something that can only do good even though these drugs are prescription-only precisely because we have every reason to think they will be riskier than alcohol—which we’re still basically happy to let people manage for themselves.
Doctors, you see, provide a risk-laundering service to companies. In fact, making drugs available through doctors is a way to hide significant hazards such as liver failure or lung cancer, on average for 10-to-15 years from the time people begin to report them first, and claim that their liver failure or lung cancer stems from the treatment.
Indeed, even after FDA puts a black box warning on alcohol or nicotine, most doctors will still deny that this risk happens.

In Pharmageddon, you describe compellingly the dilemma that general practitioners and psychiatrists face today, given the many targets they’re given and the guidelines they’re told to follow relative to their immediate issue of responding quickly, effectively, and safely to patient needs. I don’t know how closely you’re following the UK and U.S. debates about DSM-5 and ICD-11, editions that obviously will be central to determining future treatment patterns and goals, but how in your opinion can caregivers work around that dilemma, even diminish it in their work?

I think DSM-5 and ICD-11 are not at the heart of the problem. One reason for thinking this is that the key problems apply to all of medicine rather than to just mental health. DSM-5 is an example of a measurement technology, like DXA scans or peak flow meters, that generates problems for doctors to which a drug becomes an answer.
The deeper problem, as I mentioned above, is the combination of product patents, prescription-only status, and the use of clinical trials as a means of determining efficacy—in particular, when the data from those trials are not made available. This creates a perfect product for companies, with a perfect consumer (doctors) and the perfect raw material (trials), which industry can manipulate to mean whatever they want them to mean. It all adds up to the perfect market, or the perfect perversion of a market, depending on your point of view.


Sonntag, 28. Februar 2010

Newsweek: “Depressing News about Antidepressants”

Dear reader,

some days ago I received an email about a story in Newsweek on Irving Kirsch new book  "The Emperors new Drugs"  (see Link). He has uncovered a well know dirty little secret, that antidepressant are little more than active placebos. People sometime get betten when they take them because they think they get better. You get the same effect if you take some other medicine that has strong side effects not necessary an antidepressant. It is almost like in the fairy tale “The emperors new clothes”.

For more than twenty years the public was deceived about the efficiency of antidepressants. Now the truth is coming to the fore…

newsweek-print-logo

The Depressing News About Antidepressants

Studies suggest that the popular drugs are no more effective than a placebo. In fact, they may be worse.

AntiDepressants-FE01-wide-horizontal

By Sharon Begley | NEWSWEEK 

Published Jan 29, 2010

From the magazine issue dated Feb 8, 2010

Although the year is young, it has already brought my first moral dilemma. In early January a friend mentioned that his New Year's resolution was to beat his chronic depression once and for all. Over the years he had tried a medicine chest's worth of antidepressants, but none had really helped in any enduring way, and when the side effects became so unpleasant that he stopped taking them, the withdrawal symptoms (cramps, dizziness, headaches) were torture. Did I know of any research that might help him decide whether a new antidepressant his doctor recommended might finally lift his chronic darkness at noon?

 

The moral dilemma was this: oh, yes, I knew of 20-plus years of research on antidepressants, from the old tricyclics to the newer selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) that target serotonin (Zoloft, Paxil, and the granddaddy of them all, Prozac, as well as their generic descendants) to even newer ones that also target norepinephrine (Effexor, Wellbutrin). The research had shown that antidepressants help about three quarters of people with depression who take them, a consistent finding that serves as the basis for the oft-repeated mantra "There is no question that the safety and efficacy of antidepressants rest on solid scientific evidence," as psychiatry professor Richard Friedman of Weill Cornell Medical College recently wrote in The New York Times. But ever since a seminal study in 1998, whose findings were reinforced by landmark research in The Journal of the American Medical Association last month, that evidence has come with a big asterisk. Yes, the drugs are effective, in that they lift depression in most patients. But that benefit is hardly more than what patients get when they, unknowingly and as part of a study, take a dummy pill—a placebo. As more and more scientists who study depression and the drugs that treat it are concluding, that suggests that antidepressants are basically expensive Tic Tacs.

Hence the moral dilemma. The placebo effect—that is, a medical benefit you get from an inert pill or other sham treatment—rests on the holy trinity of belief, expectation, and hope. But telling someone with depression who is being helped by antidepressants, or who (like my friend) hopes to be helped, threatens to topple the whole house of cards. Explain that it's all in their heads, that the reason they're benefiting is the same reason why Disney's Dumbo could initially fly only with a feather clutched in his trunk—believing makes it so—and the magic dissipates like fairy dust in a windstorm. So rather than tell my friend all this, I chickened out. Sure, I said, there's lots of research showing that a new kind of antidepressant might help you. Come, let me show you the studies on PubMed.

It seems I am not alone in having moral qualms about blowing the whistle on antidepressants. That first analysis, in 1998, examined 38 manufacturer-sponsored studies involving just over 3,000 depressed patients. The authors, psychology researchers Irving Kirsch and Guy Sapirstein of the University of Connecticut, saw—as everyone else had—that patients did improve, often substantially, on SSRIs, tricyclics, and even MAO inhibitors, a class of antidepressants that dates from the 1950s. This improvement, demonstrated in scores of clinical trials, is the basis for the ubiquitous claim that antidepressants work. But when Kirsch compared the improvement in patients taking the drugs with the improvement in those taking dummy pills—clinical trials typically compare an experimental drug with a placebo—he saw that the difference was minuscule. Patients on a placebo improved about 75 percent as much as those on drugs. Put another way, three quarters of the benefit from antidepressants seems to be a placebo effect. "We wondered, what's going on?" recalls Kirsch, who is now at the University of Hull in England. "These are supposed to be wonder drugs and have huge effects."

The study's impact? The number of Americans taking antidepressants doubled in a decade, from 13.3 million in 1996 to 27 million in 2005.

To be sure, the drugs have helped tens of millions of people, and Kirsch certainly does not advocate that patients suffering from depression stop taking the drugs. On the contrary. But they are not necessarily the best first choice. Psychotherapy, for instance, works for moderate, severe, and even very severe depression. And although for some patients, psychotherapy in combination with an initial course of prescription antidepressants works even better, the question is, how do the drugs work? Kirsch's study and, now, others conclude that the lion's share of the drugs' effect comes from the fact that patients expect to be helped by them, and not from any direct chemical action on the brain, especially for anything short of very severe depression.

As the inexorable rise in the use of antidepressants suggests, that conclusion can't hold a candle to the simplistic "antidepressants work!" (unstated corollary: "but don't ask how") message. Part of the resistance to Kirsch's findings has been due to his less-than-retiring nature. He didn't win many friends with the cheeky title of the paper, "Listening to Prozac but Hearing Placebo." Nor did it inspire confidence that the editors of the journal Prevention & Treatment ran a warning with his paper, saying it used meta-analysis "controversially." Al-though some of the six invited commentaries agreed with Kirsch, others were scathing, accusing him of bias and saying the studies he analyzed were flawed (an odd charge for defenders of antidepressants, since the studies were the basis for the Food and Drug Administration's approval of the drugs). One criticism, however, could not be refuted: Kirsch had analyzed only some studies of antidepressants. Maybe if he included them all, the drugs would emerge head and shoulders superior to placebos.

Kirsch agreed. Out of the blue, he received a letter from Thomas Moore, who was then a health-policy analyst at George Washington University. You could expand your data set, Moore wrote, by including everything drug companies sent to the FDA—published studies, like those analyzed in "Hearing Placebo," but also unpublished studies. In 1998 Moore used the Freedom of Information Act to pry such data from the FDA. The total came to 47 company-sponsored studies—on Prozac, Paxil, Zoloft, Effexor, Serzone, and Celexa—that Kirsch and colleagues then pored over. (As an aside, it turned out that about 40 percent of the clinical trials had never been published. That is significantly higher than for other classes of drugs, says Lisa Bero of the University of California, San Francisco; overall, 22 percent of clinical trials of drugs are not published. "By and large," says Kirsch, "the unpublished studies were those that had failed to show a significant benefit from taking the actual drug.") In just over half of the published and unpublished studies, he and colleagues reported in 2002, the drug alleviated depression no better than a placebo. "And the extra benefit of antidepressants was even less than we saw when we analyzed only published studies," Kirsch recalls. About 82 percent of the response to antidepressants—not the 75 percent he had calculated from examining only published studies—had also been achieved by a dummy pill.

The extra effect of real drugs wasn't much to celebrate, either. It amounted to 1.8 points on the 54-point scale doctors use to gauge the severity of depression, through questions about mood, sleep habits, and the like. Sleeping better counts as six points. Being less fidgety during the assessment is worth two points. In other words, the clinical significance of the 1.8 extra points from real drugs was underwhelming. Now Kirsch was certain. "The belief that antidepressants can cure depression chemically is simply wrong," he told me in January on the eve of the publication of his book The Emperor's New Drugs: Exploding the Anti-depressant Myth.

The 2002 study ignited a furious debate, but more and more scientists were becoming convinced that Kirsch—who had won respect for research on the placebo response and who had published scores of scientific papers—was on to something. One team of researchers wondered if antidepressants were "a triumph of marketing over science." Even defenders of antidepressants agreed that the drugs have "relatively small" effects. "Many have long been unimpressed by the magnitude of the differences observed between treatments and controls," psychology researcher Steven Hollon of Vanderbilt University and colleagues wrote—"what some of our colleagues refer to as 'the dirty little secret.' " In Britain, the agency that assesses which treatments are effective enough for the government to pay for stopped recommending antidepressants as a first-line treatment, especially for mild or moderate depression.

But if experts know that antidepressants are hardly better than placebos, few patients or doctors do. Some doctors have changed their prescribing habits, says Kirsch, but more "reacted with anger and incredulity." Understandably. For one thing, depression is a devastating, underdiagnosed, and undertreated disease. Of course doctors recoiled at the idea that such drugs might be mirages. If that were true, how were physicians supposed to help their patients?

Two other factors are at work in the widespread rejection of Kirsch's (and, now, other scientists') findings about antidepressants. First, defenders of the drugs scoff at the idea that the FDA would have approved ineffective drugs. (Simple explanation: the FDA requires two well-designed clinical trials showing a drug is more effective than a placebo. That's two, period—even if many more studies show no such effectiveness. And the size of the "more effective" doesn't much matter, as long as it is statistically significant.) Second, doctors see with their own eyes, and feel with their hearts, that the drugs lift the black cloud from many of their depressed patients. But since doctors are not exactly in the habit of prescribing dummy pills, they have no experience comparing how their patients do on them, and therefore never see that a placebo would be almost as effective as a $4 pill. "When they prescribe a treatment and it works," says Kirsch, "their natural tendency is to attribute the cure to the treatment." Hence the widespread "antidepressants work" refrain that persists to this day.

Drug companies do not dispute Kirsch's aggregate statistics. But they point out that the average is made up of some patients in whom there is a true drug effect of antidepressants and some in whom there is not. As a spokesperson for Lilly (maker of Prozac) said, "Depression is a highly individualized illness," and "not all patients respond the same way to a particular treatment." In addition, notes a spokesperson for Glaxo-Smith-Kline (maker of Paxil), the studies analyzed in the JAMA paper differ from studies GSK submitted to the FDA when it won approval for Paxil, "so it is difficult to make direct comparisons between the results. This study contributes to the extensive research that has helped to characterize the role of antidepressants," which "are an important option, in addition to counseling and lifestyle changes, for treatment of depression." A spokesperson for Pfizer, which makes Zoloft, also cited the "wealth of scientific evidence documenting [antidepressants'] effects," adding that the fact that antidepressants "commonly fail to separate from placebo" is "a fact well known by the FDA, academia, and industry." Other manufacturers pointed out that Kirsch and the JAMA authors had not studied their particular brands.

Even Kirsch's analysis, however, found that antidepressants are a little more effective than dummy pills—those 1.8 points on the depression scale. Maybe Prozac, Zoloft, Paxil, Celexa, and their cousins do have some non-placebo, chemical benefit. But the small edge of real drugs compared with placebos might not mean what it seems, Kirsch explained to me one evening from his home in Hull. Consider how research on drugs works. Patient volunteers are told they will receive either the drug or a placebo, and that neither they nor the scientists will know who is getting what. Most volunteers hope they get the drug, not the dummy pill. After taking the unknown meds for a while, some volunteers experience side effects. Bingo: a clue they're on the real drug. About 80 percent guess right, and studies show that the worse side effects a patient experiences, the more effective the drug. Patients apparently think, this drug is so strong it's making me vomit and hate sex, so it must be strong enough to lift my depression. In clinical-trial patients who figure out they're receiving the drug and not the inert pill, expectations soar.

That matters because belief in the power of a medical treatment can be self-fulfilling (that's the basis of the placebo effect). The patients who correctly guess that they're getting the real drug therefore experience a stronger placebo effect than those who get the dummy pill, experience no side effects, and are therefore disappointed. That might account for antidepressants' slight edge in effectiveness compared with a placebo, an edge that derives not from the drugs' molecules but from the hopes and expectations that patients in studies feel when they figure out they're receiving the real drug.

The boy who said the emperor had no clothes didn't endear himself to his fellow subjects, and Kirsch has fared little better. A nascent collaboration with a scientist at a medical school ended in 2002 when the scientist was warned not to submit a grant proposal with Kirsch if he ever wanted to be funded again. Four years later, another scientist wrote a paper questioning the effectiveness of antidepressants, citing Kirsch's work. It was published in a prestigious journal. That ordinarily brings accolades. Instead, his department chair dressed him down and warned him not to become too involved with Kirsch.

But the question of whether antidepressants—which in 2008 had sales of $9.6 billion in the U.S., reported the consulting firm IMS Health—have any effect other than through patients' belief in them was too important to scare researchers off. Proponents of the drugs have found themselves making weaker and weaker claims. Their last stand is that antidepressants are more effective than a placebo in patients suffering the most severe depression.

So concluded the JAMA study in January. In an analysis of six large experiments in which, as usual, depressed patients received either a placebo or an active drug, the true drug effect—that is, in addition to the placebo effect—was "nonexistent to negligible" in patients with mild, moderate, and even severe depression. Only in patients with very severe symptoms (scoring 23 or above on the standard scale) was there a statistically significant drug benefit. Such patients account for about 13 percent of people with depression. "Most people don't need an active drug," says Vanderbilt's Hollon, a coauthor of the study. "For a lot of folks, you're going to do as well on a sugar pill or on conversations with your physicians as you will on medication. It doesn't matter what you do; it's just the fact that you're doing something." But people with very severe depression are different, he believes. "My personal view is the placebo effect gets you pretty far, but for those with very severe, more chronic conditions, it's harder to knock down and placebos are less adequate," says Hollon. Why that should be remains a mystery, admits coauthor Robert DeRubeis of the University of Pennsylvania.

Like every scientist who has stepped into the treacherous waters of antidepressant research, Hollon, DeRubeis, and their colleagues are keenly aware of the disconnect between evidence and public impression. "Prescribers, policy-makers, and consumers may not be aware that the efficacy of [antidepressants] largely has been established on the basis of studies that have included only those individuals with more severe forms of depression," something drug ads don't mention, they write. People with anything less than very severe depression "derive little specific pharmacological benefit from taking medications. Pending findings contrary to those reported here … efforts should be made to clarify to clinicians and prospective patients that … there is little evidence to suggest that [antidepressants] produce specific pharmacological benefit for the majority of patients."

Right about here, people scowl and ask how anti-depressants—especially those that raise the brain's levels of serotonin—can possibly have no direct chemical effect on the brain. Surely raising serotonin levels should right the synapses' "chemical imbalance" and lift depression. Unfortunately, the serotonin-deficit theory of depression is built on a foundation of tissue paper. How that came to be is a story in itself, but the basics are that in the 1950s scientists discovered, serendipitously, that a drug called iproniazid seemed to help some people with depression. Iproniazid increases brain levels of serotonin and norepinephrine. Ergo, low levels of those neurotransmitters must cause depression. More than 50 years on, the presumed effectiveness of antidepressants that act this way remains the chief support for the chemical-imbalance theory of depression. Absent that effectiveness, the theory hasn't a leg to stand on. Direct evidence doesn't exist. Lowering people's serotonin levels does not change their mood. And a new drug, tianeptine, which is sold in France and some other countries (but not the U.S.), turns out to be as effective as Prozac-like antidepressants that keep the synapses well supplied with serotonin. The mechanism of the new drug? It lowers brain levels of serotonin. "If depression can be equally affected by drugs that increase serotonin and by drugs that decrease it," says Kirsch, "it's hard to imagine how the benefits can be due to their chemical activity."

Perhaps antidepressants would be more effective at higher doses? Unfortunately, in 2002 Kirsch and colleagues found that high doses are hardly more effective than low ones, improving patients' depression-scale rating an average of 9.97 points vs. 9.57 points—a difference that is not statistically significant. Yet many doctors increase doses for patients who do not respond to a lower one, and many patients report improving as a result. There's a study of that, too. When researchers gave such nonresponders a higher dose, 72 percent got much better, their symptoms dropping by 50 percent or more. The catch? Only half the patients really got a higher dose. The rest, unknowingly, got the original, "ineffective" dose. It is hard to see the 72 percent who got much better on ersatz higher doses as the result of anything but the power of expectation: the doctor upped my dose, so I believe I'll get better.

Something similar may explain why some patients who aren't helped by one antidepressant do better on a second, or a third. This is often explained as "matching" patient to drug, and seemed to be confirmed by a 2006 federal study called STAR*D. Patients still suffering from depression after taking one drug were switched to a second; those who were still not better were switched to a third drug, and even a fourth. No placebos were used. At first blush, the results offered a ray of hope: 37 percent of the patients got better on the first drug, 19 percent more on their second, 6 percent more improved on their third try, and 5 percent more on their fourth. (Half of those who recovered relapsed within a year, however.)

So does STAR*D validate the idea that the key to effective treatment of depression is matching the patient to the drug? Maybe. Or maybe people improved in rounds two, three, and four because depression sometimes lifts due to changes in people's lives, or because levels of depression tend to rise and fall over time. With no one in STAR*D receiving a placebo, it is not possible to conclude with certainty that the improvements in rounds two, three, and four were because patients switched to a drug that was more effective for them. Comparable numbers might have improved if they had switched to a placebo. But STAR*D did not test for that, and so cannot rule it out.

It's tempting to look at the power of the placebo effect to alleviate depression and stick an "only" in front of it—as in, the drugs work only through the placebo effect. But there is nothing "only" about the placebo response. It can be surprisingly enduring, as a 2008 study found: "The widely held belief that the placebo response in depression is short-lived appears to be based largely on intuition and perhaps wishful thinking," scientists wrote in the Journal of Psychiatric Research. The strength of the placebo response drives drug companies nuts, since it makes showing the superiority of a new drug much harder. There is a strong placebo component in the response to drugs for pain, asthma, irritable-bowel syndrome, skin conditions such as contact dermatitis, and even Parkinson's disease. But compared with the placebo component of antidepressants, the placebo response accounts for a smaller fraction of the benefit from drugs for those disorders—on the order of 50 percent for analgesics, for instance.

Which returns us to the moral dilemma. In any year, an estimated 13.1 million to 14.2 million American adults suffer from clinical depression. At least 32 million will have the disease at some point in their life. Many of the 57 percent who receive treatment (the rest do not) are helped by medication. For that benefit to continue, they need to believe in their pills. Even Kirsch warns—in boldface type in his book, which is in stores this week—that patients on antidepressants not suddenly stop taking them. That can cause serious withdrawal symptoms, including twitches, tremors, blurred vision, and nausea—as well as depression and anxiety. Yet Kirsch is well aware that his book may have the same effect on patients as dropping the magic feather did for Dumbo: without it, the little elephant began crashing to earth. Friends and colleagues who believe Kirsch is right ask why he doesn't just shut up, since publicizing the finding that the effectiveness of antidepressants is almost entirely due to people's hopes and expectations will undermine that effectiveness.

It's all well and good to point out that psychotherapy is more effective than either pills or placebos, with dramatically lower relapse rates. But there's the little matter of reality. In the U.S., most patients with depression are treated by primary-care doctors, not psychiatrists. The latter are in short supply, especially outside cities and especially for children and adolescents. Some insurance plans discourage such care, and some psychiatrists do not accept insurance. Maybe keeping patients in the dark about the ineffectiveness of antidepressants, which for many are their only hope, is a kindness.

Or maybe not. As shown by the explicit criticism of drug companies by the authors of the recent JAMA paper, more and more scientists believe it is time to abandon the "don't ask, don't tell" policy of not digging too deeply into the reasons for the effectiveness of antidepressants. Maybe it is time to pull back the curtain and see the wizard for what he is. As for Kirsch, he insists that it is important to know that much of the benefit of antidepressants is a placebo effect. If placebos can make people better, then depression can be treated without drugs that come with serious side effects, not to mention costs. Wider recognition that antidepressants are a pharmaceutical version of the emperor's new clothes, he says, might spur patients to try other treatments. "Isn't it more important to know the truth?" he asks. Based on the impact of his work so far, it's hard to avoid answering, "Not to many people."

With Sarah Kliff

Find this article at http://www.newsweek.com/id/232781

© 2010

Sonntag, 24. Januar 2010

Arzneimittelprüfer Sawicki erhält keinen neuen Vertrag




Liebe Leser,

sie haben sicherlich auch schon davon gehört, dass der Leiter des IQWiG, Peter Sawitzki, keien neuen vertrag bekommt und im August gehen muss. Es ist viel die rede von politischer Einflußnahme durch die Politiker der FDP, die ihrer Klientel eien lästigen Kritiker vom Hals schaffen will. Da stellt sich nach dem Skandal der Steuererleichterungen für die Hoteliers und der Spende eines prominenten Hotelunternehmers die Frage, wieviel die Pharmaindustrie wohl der FDP gespendet hat?

Wie ist eure Meinung dazu? Lesen sie dazu den folgenden Bericht aus Stern.de (http://www.stern.de/gesundheit/iqwig-chef-sawicki-ein-opfer-der-pharmalobby-1537740.html)



Ein Opfer der Pharmalobby

Er ist der Schrecken der Pharmaindustrie: Peter Sawicki, Leiter des Instituts für Qualität und Wirtschaftlichkeit im Gesundheitswesen. Heute hat der Stiftungsrat beschlossen, dass er Ende August seinen Hut nehmen muss. Ein Lehrstück in Sachen Lobbyismus. Von Lea Wolz, Lutz Kinkel und Nina Bublitz.





Als Pharmakritiker hat er viele Feinde. Nun ist er offiziell über eine "Dienstwagenaffäre" gestolpert: Peter Sawicki, der Leiter des Instituts für Qualität und Wirtschaftlichkeit im Gesundheitswesen (Iqwig), muss gehen. Der Vertrag des 52-jährigen Wissenschaftlers laufe zum 31. August aus, teilten Vorstand und Stiftungsrat des Instituts am Freitag in Berlin mit. Er hätte zwar gerne noch weitergemacht, höre aber zum 31. August auf, sagte Sawicki.





Für den Verbraucherschutz ist das eine deutliche Niederlage, die Pharmaindustrie dürfte dagegen jubeln. Sawicki, der das Iqwig seit 2004 leitet, war wegen seiner pharmakritischen Haltung bei den Arzneimittelherstellern von Anfang an heftig umstritten. Auch in der industrienahen schwarz-gelben Koalition hatte er wenig Freunde. Schon im Koalitionsvertrag sprachen sich die Fraktionen von Union und FDP dafür aus, die Arbeit des Instituts zu überprüfen - und es industriefreundlicher auszurichten. Künftig soll es stärker die Interessen der Pharmaindustrie und die Erfahrung der Ärzte in den Krankenhäusern berücksichtigen. Kurz gesagt: Es scheint so, als habe die schwarz-gelbe Regierung mit dem Fall Sawicki ein weiteres Lehrstück ihrer Klientelpolitik gegeben. Nutznießer dieses Krimis um einen der letzten unbestechlichen Kritiker der Arzneimittelindustrie ist eben diese.


Ein Dorn im Auge der Pharmaindustrie
Den Arzneimittelherstellern ist das Iqwig schon seit seiner Gründung im Zuge der Gesundheitsreform 2004 ein Dorn im Auge. Denn mit Pseudoinnovationen, die das Budget der Krankenkassen belasten, dem Patienten aber wenig bringen, lässt sich viel Geld aus dem Gesundheitssystem ziehen. Um dem entgegenzutreten, prüft das Iqwig Studien zu Arzneimitteln und untersucht als "Medikamenten-TÜV", ob Therapien und die oft teuren Medikamente auch nützlich sind. Andernfalls können sie aus dem Leistungskatalog der Krankenkassen gestrichen werden. Für einen Hersteller kann das Verluste in mehrstelliger Millionenhöhe bedeuten.
So machte sich das Institut bei der Pharmaindustrie wenig beliebt, als es entschied, dass Analoginsuline nicht besser sind als herkömmliches Humaninsulin. Stattdessen warnten die Medikamentenprüfer immer wieder vor Nebenwirkungen. Im Juni 2009 stellte das Iqwig fest, dass ein künstliches Insulin bei Diabetikern sogar Krebs auslösen könnte. Auch das Medikament Memamtin, das bei Alzheimer verordnet wird, fand nicht den Segen der Arzneimittelprüfer. Ein Beleg für den Nutzen fehle, hieß es aus dem Iqwig. Wer so handelt, macht sich viele Feinde - und sollte sich daher besser nach allen Seiten absichern.
Mächtigster Pharmakritiker stürzt über Benzin für den Rasenmäher
Offiziell ist der 52-jährige Sawicki mit den grau gewellten Haaren und den feinen Gesichtszügen nun allerdings über sein Auslagengebaren gestolpert. Unter anderem hat sich der harte Pharmakritiker durch den Abschluss von zwei Leasing-Verträgen für Dienstwagen angreifbar gemacht, da er zuvor den Vorstand nicht informiert hatte. Sawicki wies die Vorwürfe zurück. Ein von ihm in Auftrag gegebenes Rechtsgutachten kommt ebenfalls zu dem Schluss, dass er mit den geleasten Fahrzeugen - einem Audi Q7 und einem Audi Q5 - nicht gegen seinen Dienstvertrag verstoßen habe. Sawickis Dienstwagen hätten stets ordnungsgemäß im Haushaltsplan des Iqwig gestanden. Die Vorstandsmitglieder und der Stiftungsrat waren mithin darüber informiert.
Daneben wird dem Leiter des Iqwig vorgeworfen, sich auch bei einzelnen privaten Abrechnungen in einer Höhe von insgesamt knapp 1200 Euro - unter anderem für Rasenmäher-Benzin, Maut- und Parkgebühren - regelwidrig verhalten zu haben. 2007 stand Sawicki bereits in der Kritik, da er als Chef des Iqwig an das private Institut seiner Frau einen Auftrag vergeben hatte. Allerdings gab es auch Beschwerden, das Iqwig sei nicht effizient und arbeite wissenschaftlich ungenau.





"Kritische Arzneimittelbewertung wird geschwächt"
"Die Entscheidung, Sawicki zu entlassen, ist eindeutig politisch motiviert", sagt der Herausgeber des pharmakritischen "Arznei-Telegramms", Wolfgang Becker-Brüser zu stern.de. Die kritische Arzneimittelbeurteilung werde dadurch entscheidend geschwächt. "Sawicki hat sich bei seiner Beurteilung von Medikamenten immer auf wissenschaftliche Studien gestützt und sich nicht auf einen Handel mit den Pharmafirmen eingelassen", meint der Pharmazeut und Arzt. Bei der sogenannten evidenzbasierten Medizin muss ein Medikament in randomisierten und kontrollierten klinischen Studien beweisen, dass es besser ist als die bereits erhältlichen. Nun werde wohl ein industriefreundlicherer Kandidat den Posten erhalten, befürchtet Becker-Brüser. Zum Schaden der Verbraucher: "Langfristig treibt das die Krankenkassenbeiträge wieder hoch."





Auch international findet der Fall Sawicki Beachtung. Das renommierte Wissenschaftsmagazin "Science" berichtet in der aktuellen Ausgabe über die Entscheidung, die zu Druckschluss noch nicht gefallen war. Zu Wort kommt Gerd Antes, Direktor des deutschen Cochrane-Zentrums, der meint, dass Sawickis Ausscheiden die Arbeit des Iqwig untergraben würde. Es sei Teil einer politischen Strategie, die strengen Verfahren bei neuen Medikamenten und Medizinprodukten zu lockern.

Vorwürfe "lächerlich" und "vorgeschoben"
Auch SPD, Grüne und Linke warnen, durch die Ablösung solle der Einfluss der Pharmaindustrie wachsen. Die Vorwürfe an Sawicki hinsichtlich der Amtsführung seien "lächerlich" und "vorgeschoben", sagte der SPD-Gesundheitsexperte Karl Lauterbach zu stern.de. Es sei von Anfang an klar gewesen, dass Union und FDP dass Institut nicht nach den Regeln der evidenzbasierten Medizin laufen lassen wollten. "Sawicki war für die Neuausrichtung des Instituts der falsche Mann. Deswegen musste er entsorgt werden", kritisiert Lauterbach.
Bei den Grünen sieht man das ähnlich: "Es ist schäbig, Sawicki über die Spesenabrechnung loswerden zu wollen. Weil es klar ist, dass der Hintergrund ein anderer ist, nämlich ein politischer: Man will einen Pharmakritiker kaltstellen", sagte Birgitt Bender, gesundheitspolitische Sprecherin der Grünen, zu stern.de. "Wenn durch diese Personalentscheidung die Arbeit des Instituts weichgespült werden soll, werden das die Versicherten teuer bezahlen. Denn dann würden Mondpreise für Medikamente mit wenig Nutzen verlangt und die falschen Anreize für die Pharmaindustrie gesetzt." Die gesundheitspolitische Sprecherin der Linksfraktion, Martina Bunge, erklärte, mit der Entscheidung würden "alle enttäuscht, die sich ein unabhängiges Gegengewicht zur Übermacht der Pharmakonzerne wünschen".
Vorwürfen, die Arzneiprüfungen lockern zu wollen ist die Bundesregierung bereits entgegengetreten. Das Iqwig leiste eine notwendige und gute Arbeit, sagte Gesundheitsstaatssekretär Stefan Kapferer. "Eine Lockerung der Prüfregeln soll es nicht geben." Für den Posten als neuer Chef des Iqwig ist stern.de-Informationen zufolge unter anderem Leo Hansen von der Kassenärztlichen Vereinigung Nordrhein im Gespräch.

Samstag, 26. Dezember 2009

Peter Rost Gets A Little Help From The Government

peter-rost-4

This Blog is from Ed Silvermann from Pharmalot.

For those who may not remember, Peter Rost is the controversial gadfly and former Pfizer exec who is locked in a whistleblower lawsuit with the big drugmaker over his allegations that Genotropin, a human growth hormone, was marketed for unapproved uses, such as combating aging in adults and treating short stature in children. Rost worked for Pharmacia, which was bought by Pfizer.

Although the US Justice Department failed to join his lawsuit, Rost is pressing on and his case is being closely watched. Last year, he cited numerous instances in Indiana and Kentucky in which Genotropin marketing may have violated the False Claims Act. Providing such detail was needed for the case to proceed and his efforts may serve as a template for other whistleblowers who are stymied by federal judges seeking details that are very hard to come by.

Rost showed that is possible to clear rule 9b of the False Claims Act, which refers to fraud claims with “sufficient specificity,” another way of saying a whistleblower must provide very specific info about false claims a drugmaker submitted to the government for payment. This may include amounts charged, drugs prescribed, patient diagnosis and individuals involved in billing, a level of detail that can be very hard to obtain, given patient privacy laws such as HIPAA.

Pfizer has repeatedly moved to dismiss the case, but yesterday the Justice Department filed an amicus brief, stating it “remains a real party interest” in the litigation, even though it chose not to intervene. The reason for its brief was, essentially, to shoot down Pfizer’s interpretation of the Medicaid Act and the False Claims Act. “The United States has a keen interest in the development of the law in this area and in the correct application of the law in this, and similar, cases,” the DOJ writes. In doing so, the government gave Rost a lift, even with a caveat that no position was taken on the merits of his claims (here is the brief).

In explaining its views, the DOJ argues that “summary judgment should not be granted simply because someone other than the defendant submitted the false claims, a prior authorization scheme existed, or physicians deny that they were influenced by kickbacks.” The DOJ also contends that circumstantial evidence is sufficient to prove that the False Claims Act can be violated.

The DOJ rejects several Pfizer arguments. For instance, the DOJ writes that “…the fact that Pharmacia may have caused false claims to be submitted through the actions of pharmacists is irrelevant. As long as the submission of the false claims was the foreseeable result of its illegal kickbacks, it has caused the submission of false claims and is liable under the FCA.”

Pfizer has been arguing that Rost’s lawsuit should be tossed because the drugmaker itself had not submitted any claims to Medicaid that could be construed as false and Genotropin was already listed under state prior authorization programs, among other things.

 

For those who may not remember, Peter Rost is the controversial gadfly and former Pfizer exec who is locked in a whistleblower lawsuit with the big drugmaker over his allegations that Genotropin, a human growth hormone, was marketed for unapproved uses, such as combating aging in adults and treating short stature in children. Rost worked for Pharmacia, which was bought by Pfizer.

Although the US Justice Department failed to join his lawsuit, Rost is pressing on and his case is being closely watched. Last year, he cited numerous instances in Indiana and Kentucky in which Genotropin marketing may have violated the False Claims Act. Providing such detail was needed for the case to proceed and his efforts may serve as a template for other whistleblowers who are stymied by federal judges seeking details that are very hard to come by.

Rost showed that is possible to clear rule 9b of the False Claims Act, which refers to fraud claims with “sufficient specificity,” another way of saying a whistleblower must provide very specific info about false claims a drugmaker submitted to the government for payment. This may include amounts charged, drugs prescribed, patient diagnosis and individuals involved in billing, a level of detail that can be very hard to obtain, given patient privacy laws such as HIPAA.

Pfizer has repeatedly moved to dismiss the case, but yesterday the Justice Department filed an amicus brief, stating it “remains a real party interest” in the litigation, even though it chose not to intervene. The reason for its brief was, essentially, to shoot down Pfizer’s interpretation of the Medicaid Act and the False Claims Act. “The United States has a keen interest in the development of the law in this area and in the correct application of the law in this, and similar, cases,” the DOJ writes. In doing so, the government gave Rost a lift, even with a caveat that no position was taken on the merits of his claims (here is the brief).

In explaining its views, the DOJ argues that “summary judgment should not be granted simply because someone other than the defendant submitted the false claims, a prior authorization scheme existed, or physicians deny that they were influenced by kickbacks.” The DOJ also contends that circumstantial evidence is sufficient to prove that the False Claims Act can be violated.

The DOJ rejects several Pfizer arguments. For instance, the DOJ writes that “…the fact that Pharmacia may have caused false claims to be submitted through the actions of pharmacists is irrelevant. As long as the submission of the false claims was the foreseeable result of its illegal kickbacks, it has caused the submission of false claims and is liable under the FCA.”

Pfizer has been arguing that Rost’s lawsuit should be tossed because the drugmaker itself had not submitted any claims to Medicaid that could be construed as false and Genotropin was already listed under state prior authorization programs, among other things.

 

Codes R not us.

American way of life is something else, that includes your justice system and everything else. That is why things change so slowely and with such pain. Look at Obama’s noble goal to give everyone health care in the richest country in the world, how dificult the process is. Here we have a company Pfizer that already paid $2.300.000.000.00 in fines for doing all kinds of illegal stuff for years, including offlabel promo (their routine way to make extra $$) and the DOJ is not going after them on this one. They did it for sure so what is the problem? We all hope Peter wins. Yes he deserves it as much as the other successful WBers.

Samstag, 24. Oktober 2009

Pfizer und das FBI

FBI_Pfizer

Liebe Leser,

wenn sie mehr über die Ermittlungen des FBI gegen Pfizer wegen der illegalen Vermarktung von Bextra und Celebrex erfahren wollen und die 2,3 Mrd. Strafzahlung an die US Regierung, so sollten sie hier mal nachschauen :http://www.fbi.gov/page2/sept09/pfizer_settlement_090209.html

http://www.fbi.gov/inside/archive/inside090309.htm

Sonntag, 11. Oktober 2009

Pfizers secret Files , Risk of SUICIDE was evident when Zoloft was registered in Germany

 

Dear Mr. Herxheimer,

you may remember that  3 years ago you accompanied me and Prof. Müller-Oerlinghausen when we visited the Europeam Medical Agency EMEA to read some documents on SSRI antidepressants.

After more than three years I succeeded to get access to the files regarding the registration of Zoloft in Germany at the german regulatory agency BfArM.

My lawyer, Prof. Müller-Oerlinghausen from the Drug Commission of the German Medical Association and I have looked into the files of the german medical authorities and have found documents proving my claim, that the increased risk of suicide was evident when ZOLOFT was registered in Germany in 1996. The data from all clinical trials show that the rate of suicide among the patients taking sertraline was statistically significant higher than among the patients getting placebos or a different antidepressant. Also the death rate in the sertraline group was significantly higher. But the german expert ignored the data and claimed instead that sertraline even reduces the risk of suicide.

I hesitated for some time to give the documents to the press and to the public because the german authorites did not allow me to give them to anybody. But the public has a right to know the facts! The crucial pages are page 50 to 53 of the expert report (page 1-3 of the file Gutachten_1995_03.pdf) and the tables of the listing of all deaths. They show that until 1.sept. 1993  16940 patients taking part in medical studies took sertraline and 0.09% (=15 patients) were suicidal. In the control group of 9016 patients only 0.02% were suicidal (=2 patients). This diffenence ist statistically significant with more than 99 percent probability!

The tables show that among the 46 deaths of all clinical trials 34 patients had taken sertraline and among them 10 committed suicide (9 official suicides and one patients poisioned himself with carbon-monoxide). In the control group only one patient committed suicide.

All these data were ignored by the german expert, a very prominent german psychiatrist, who has written the report. Instead he claimed that Zoloft even reduces the risk of suicide. This report was the medical basis for the german medical agency to approve Zoloft in germany in 1996.

I will keep you informed.

best regards,

Lothar Schröder

Mittwoch, 10. Dezember 2008

Frontal21 German TV-Documentary: Big Pharma SSRI Marketing Push




On Tuesday, December 9, German TV ZDF will broadcast a hard hitting documentary about corrupt pharmaceutical company practices.One issue that will be addressed for the first time is: Why did the German drug regulatory agency (BGA) approve Prozac in 1991 after denying Eli Lilly the license to market the drug in Germany in 1984 without explicit label warnings about the increased suicide risk the drug poses ? May 25, 1984 Internal memorandum from Eli Lilly regarding the company's efforts to obtain a marketing license for Prozac in Germany states: "During the treatment with the preparation [fluoxetine] 16 suicide attempts were made, 2 of these with success. As patients with a risk of suicide were excluded from the studies, it is probable that this high proportion can be attributed to an action of the preparation in the sence (sic) of an deterioration of the clinical condition, which reached its lowest point." [PZ281]Furthermore, Lilly's own documents reveal that a 1988 review of clinical trials found that 38% of patients taking Prozac compared to 19% of patients on placebo experienced "activation," which is linked by the FDA in current labeling to violent and suicidal behavior. [PZ-477] . See: Eli Lilly internal documents: What do They Reveal? [http://www.ahrp.org/infomail/05/01/27.php]BZ281: Lilly Memo, Keitz; Bad Homburg to Corporate Headquarters, May 25, 1984, with Comment on Clinical Documentation Fluoxetine from BGA (Germany) rejecting Prozac for marketing.PZ-477: Lilly Memo. Activation and Sedation in Fluoxetine Clinical Trials, 1988See: Lenzer, J. FDA to Review "Missing" Drug Company Documents, BMJ (formerly British Medical Journal), January 1, 2005. [http://bmj.bmjjournals.com/cgi/content/full/330/7481/7]
























From: Lothar Schröder
Sent: Saturday, December 06, 2008 6:32
AMTo: Veracare

Subject: German TV-Documentary on SSRI and psychotopic Drugs

Dear Vera,

Since the death of my wife I am trying to inform the German public about the risk and dangers of SSRI-antidepressants. My wife took ZOLOFT 3 years ago for only 11 days. Two days before her death the medication was stopped abruptly. A few months after her death the European Commission made it mandatory for European license holder of SSRI- antidepressants to include a warning about the risk of suicide for children and adolescents in the product information and package insert. Before it, the risk of suicide was not even listed in the product information and package insert.I have tried to press criminal charges against Pfizer but the German courts have put it down. I have also informed the media about it. Now next Tuesday (09. December 2008, 21:00 hour German time on ZDF) the German TV ZDF will broadcast a documentary about the criminal practices by the big pharmacy companies to press the SSRI antidepressant to the European market. The German regulatory agency BGA (Bundesgesundheitsamt) knew about the risk of suicide of the SSRI Prozac and refused to admit it to the German market: first in 1985 and then 3 years later in 1988. But finally Prozac was admitted to the German market in 1991. Zoloft and the other SSRI antidepressants followed. Last Tuesday, Frontal 21 has broadcast a 6-minute long report about Prozac and Zoloft (see: [http://frontal21.zdf.de/ZDFde/inhalt/19/0,1872,7486227,00.html] ). I hope that this report and the documentary will put real pressure on our politicians and on our regulatory agency. Why was Prozac approved in 1991 although the german BGA knew about the risk of suicide since 1984? Maybe you want to inform the readers of your infomail - I am one of them for many years- and in your blog about the documentary. It will be seen in all German speaking countries in Europe. Thank you very much.

Best regards,

Lothar Schröder